New Scientist reports on a study of Oxytocin and how it gets overhyped on the internet.
« July 2010 | Main | September 2010 »
New Scientist reports on a study of Oxytocin and how it gets overhyped on the internet.
Posted by Adam Kolber on 08/31/2010 at 03:13 AM | Permalink | Comments (1)
![]() |
Posted by Adam Kolber on 08/30/2010 at 03:06 AM | Permalink | Comments (0)
From the most recent issue of Neuroethics:
by Alfred Mele
Abstract:
This article describes three experiments that would advance our understanding of the import of data already generated by scientific work on free will and related issues. All three can be conducted with existing technology. The first concerns how reliable a predictor of behavior a certain segment of type I and type II RPs is. The second focuses on the timing of conscious experiences in Libet-style studies. The third concerns the effectiveness of conscious implementation intentions. The discussion of first two experiments highlights some important problems with certain inferences made on the basis of existing data in scientific work on free will. The discussion of the third calls attention to powerful evidence that conscious intentions sometimes are among the causes of corresponding actions. This evidence has been largely ignored in the literature on free will.
Posted by Adam Kolber on 08/27/2010 at 02:22 AM | Permalink | Comments (0)
From the most recent issue of Neuroethics:
Moral Aspects of Psychiatric Diagnosis: the Cluster B Personality Disorders
By Marga Reimer
Abstract:
Medical professionals, including mental health professionals, largely agree that moral judgment should be kept out of clinical settings. The rationale is simple: moral judgment has the capacity to impair clinical judgment in ways that could harm the patient. However, when the patient is suffering from a "Cluster B" personality disorder, keeping moral judgment out of the clinic might appear impossible, not only in practice but also in theory. For the diagnostic criteria associated with these particular disorders (Antisocial, Borderline, Histrionic, Narcissistic) are expressed in overtly moral language. I consider three proposals for dealing with this problem. The first is to eliminate the Cluster B disorders from the DSM on the grounds that they are moral, rather than mental, disorders. The second is to replace the morally laden language of the diagnostic criteria with morally neutral language. The third is to disambiguate the notion of moral judgment so as to respect the distinction between having morally disvalued traits and having moral responsibility for those traits. Sensitivity to this distinction enables the clinician, at least in theory, to employ morally laden diagnostic criteria without adopting the sort of morally judgmental (and potentially harmful) attitude that results from the tacit presumption of moral responsibility. I argue against the first two proposals and in favor of the third. In doing so, I appeal to Grice's distinction between conventional and conversational implicature. I close with a few brief remarks on the irony of retaining overtly moral language in an ostensibly medical manual for the diagnosis of mental disorders.
Posted by Adam Kolber on 08/26/2010 at 02:18 AM | Permalink | Comments (0)
Recently posted to SSRN: "At the Mercy of the Prisoner Next Door: Using an Experimental Measure of Selfishness as a Criminological Tool"
MPI Collective Goods Preprint No. 2010/27THORSTEN CHMURA, University of Bonn - Faculty of Law & Economics
CHRISTOPH ENGEL, Max Planck Institute for Research on Collective Goods
MARKUS ENGLERTH, affiliation not provided to SSRN
THOMAS PITZ, University of Bonn
Do criminals maximise money? Are criminals more or less selfish than the average subject? Can prisons apply measures that reduce the degree of selfishness of their inmates? Using a tried and tested tool from experimental economics, we cast new light on these old criminological questions. In a standard dictator game, prisoners give a substantial amount, which calls for more refined versions of utility in rational choice theories of crime. Prisoners do not give less than average subjects, not even than subjects from other closely knit communities. This speaks against the idea that people commit crimes because they are excessively selfish. Finally those who receive better marks at prison school give more, as do those who improve their marks over time. This suggests that this correctional intervention also reduces selfishness.
Posted by Adam Kolber on 08/25/2010 at 02:12 AM | Permalink | Comments (0)
![]() |
Last Edition's Most Popular Article:
| ||
|
Posted by Adam Kolber on 08/23/2010 at 02:38 PM | Permalink | Comments (0)
Recently posted to SSRN:
"Implicit Bias, Election '08, and the Myth of a Post-Racial America"
JEFFREY J. RACHLINSKI, Cornell Law School
GREGORY SCOTT PARKS, District of Columbia Court of Appeals
The election of Barack Obama as the Forty-fourth President of the United States signals that the traditional modes of thinking about race in America are outmoded. Commentators and pundits have begun to suggest that the election of a Black man to the nation's highest office means that the United States has entered a post-racial era in which civil rights laws are becoming unnecessary. Although President Obama's election means that explicit, open anti-Black racism has largely faded, an analysis of the campaign's rhetoric and themes suggests that unconscious racism is alive and well. Rather than suggest a retreat from traditional civil rights protections, the 2008 election calls for maintaining and enhancing efforts to ensure that civil rights laws to address less virulent, less explicit forms of racism that persist.
Posted by Adam Kolber on 08/23/2010 at 03:27 AM | Permalink | Comments (0)
As I predicted, a lot more information has come out regarding the Marc Hauser investigation at Harvard. Apparently, a former research assistant in Hauser's lab who is no longer in the field of psychology has given The Chronicle of Higher Education a document from the investigation:
It tells the story of how research assistants became convinced that the professor was reporting bogus data and how he aggressively pushed back against those who questioned his findings or asked for verification.
. . .
The former research assistant, who provided the document on condition of anonymity, said his motivation in coming forward was to make it clear that it was solely Mr. Hauser who was responsible for the problems he observed. The former research assistant also hoped that more information might help other researchers make sense of the allegations.
The allegations seem pretty serious. Here's the news from The Chronicle (via Leiter via Mahon).
Posted by Adam Kolber on 08/20/2010 at 12:37 PM | Permalink | Comments (0)
From the most recent issue of Neuroethics:
Posted by Adam Kolber on 08/20/2010 at 02:15 AM | Permalink | Comments (0)
In June, I linked to a series in Slate on Elizabeth Loftus's work on memory. One of the pieces in Slate offered some tantalizing clues to work that she and several others have done on people's attitudes toward possible memory dampening drugs. The group's fascinating results have now been published in Applied Cognitive Psychology:
Title: Attitudes about memory dampening drugs depend on context and country
Authors: Eryn J. Newman1, Shari R. Berkowitz2, Kally J. Nelson2, Maryanne Garry1, Elizabeth F. Loftus2,*
Abstract:
When people take drugs such as propranolol in response to trauma, it may dampen their bad memories – tempering recall of a traumatic event. We examined people's attitudes toward these drugs. Americans and New Zealanders read about a hypothetical assault inserting themselves into a scenario as a victim attacked while serving on a peace keeping mission (soldier role) or while walking home from a job as a restaurant manager (civilian role). Then they told us whether they should receive a memory dampening drug, and whether they would want to take a memory dampening drug. Subjects were negatively disposed towards a memory dampening drug, but Americans who adopted the soldier role were more in favor of having access to the drug than those who adopted the civilian role. We discuss the implications of these findings in relation to an increasing trend in ‘cosmetic neurology’, medicating with the goal of enhancement, rather than therapy.
Posted by Adam Kolber on 08/18/2010 at 01:11 PM | Permalink | Comments (0)
Recently posted to SSRN:
"Seeing Through Colorblindness: Implicit Bias and the Law"
UCLA Law Review, ForthcomingJERRY KANG, University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA) - School of Law
KRISTIN LANE, Bard College Program in Psychology
Once upon a time, the central civil rights questions were indisputably normative. What did “equal justice under law” require? Did it, for example, permit segregation, or was separate never equal? This is no longer the case. Today, the central civil rights questions of our time turn also on the underlying empirics. In a post-civil rights era, in what some people exuberantly embrace as post-racial, many assume that we already live in a colorblind society. Is this in fact the case? Recent findings about implicit bias from mind scientists sharply suggest other-wise. This Article summarizes the empirical evidence that rejects facile claims of perceptual, cognitive, and behavioral colorblindness. It then calls on the law to take a “behaviorally realist” account of these findings, and maps systematically how it might do so in sensible, non-hysterical, and evidence-based ways. Recognizing that this call may be politically naive, the Article examines and answers three objections, sounding in “junk science” backlash, “hard-wired” resignation, and “rational” justification.
Posted by Adam Kolber on 08/18/2010 at 06:24 AM | Permalink | Comments (0)
![]() |
Last Edition's Most Popular Article:
| ||
|
Posted by Adam Kolber on 08/16/2010 at 02:01 AM | Permalink | Comments (0)
The New York Times has another article today about the investigation of Marc Hauser's empirical research on moral reasoning. The theme of the article is that Harvard has been not at all forthcoming about possible misconduct and the reasons for retractions/amendments to prior research papers. The net effect, according to the article, is that those working in the same field, especially Hauser's former students and co-authors, are besmirched by a cloud of uncertainty.
The article states:
Jeff Neal, a public affairs officer at Harvard, suggested in an e-mail that it was up to the federal government, which financed some of the research, to publish any report on the case. Harvard reports any findings about research misconduct to the government, he said, and “in cases where the government concludes scientific misconduct occurred, the federal agency makes those findings publicly available.”
Notice that the spokesperson's statement is vague. It makes it sound like Harvard is not empowered to release information from its investigation. True, Harvard says that reviews of faculty conduct are confidential, but does that really prevent Harvard from providing more information of some sort? I predict that we're going to learn quite a bit more about what happened. Certainly today's NYT piece and this Boston Globe editorial will increase the pressure for transparency.
Posted by Adam Kolber on 08/13/2010 at 08:37 AM | Permalink | Comments (1)
Recently posted to SSRN:
"Foreword to the Neuroscience, Law & Government Symposium"
Akron Law Review, Vol. 42, p. 681, 2009JANE CAMPBELL MORIARTY, University of Akron School of Law
This article introduces the various subjects and articles from the Neuroscience, Law and Government Symposium, published in the Akron Law Review (2009) following the Conference held at the University of Akron School of Law in September, 2009. The Conference and Symposium consider both the explosion in neuroscience research and its implications for both law and government, beginning with remarks by the keynote speaker, Professor Henry T. Greeley. A substantial portion of the symposium addresses the implications of scientists’ efforts to depict deception by means of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), including an article on the legal reliability of such evidence, the fourth and fifth amendment implications of such evidence, and the persuasiveness of such evidence with juries. The symposium also includes other varied topics, including the implications of neuroscience on the insanity defense, juvenile justice, predictions of disease or criminality, enhancing performance, and the role of neuroscience in legislation about women’s medical conditions
Posted by Adam Kolber on 08/12/2010 at 10:23 AM | Permalink | Comments (0)