Cognitive Daily has an interesting post about the aIAT (autobiographical implicit association test) when used as a method of lie detection. Looks like it can be cheated pretty easily.
« June 2009 | Main | August 2009 »
Cognitive Daily has an interesting post about the aIAT (autobiographical implicit association test) when used as a method of lie detection. Looks like it can be cheated pretty easily.
Posted by Adam Kolber on 07/31/2009 at 08:25 AM | Permalink | Comments (0) | TrackBack (0)
The piece below by Christopher Robertson (Law, Fellow at Petrie-Flom at Harvard Law School) was recently posted to SSRN. It looks very interesting:
CHRISTOPHER T. ROBERTSON, Harvard University - Harvard Law School
America spends hundreds of billions of dollars on its system of civil litigation, and expert witnesses appear in the vast majority of cases. Yet, litigants currently select and retain expert witnesses in ways that create the appearance of biased hired-guns on both sides of every case and thereby deprive factfinders of a clear view of the scientific facts. As a result, factfinders too often get it wrong, which undermines deterrence and compensation and facilitates frivolous claims and defenses. Although many have suggested that court-appointed experts could be a solution, such proposals raise concerns about accuracy and fairness and have failed to gain traction in the American system that depends on deeply-ingrained adversarial norms and incentives.
Drawing on the notion of blind research used in the sciences and the veil of ignorance in political theory, this Article offers a novel and feasible reform that brings unbiased experts to factfinders as a rational strategy of self-interested litigants within the adversarial system. If litigants use an intermediary to randomly select qualified experts to render opinions without knowledge of their sponsors, some biases can be eliminated and those litigants will win more cases. A probabilistic analysis reveals that the result will be greater accuracy of litigation outcomes compared to the status quo and compared to litigation with court-appointed experts. A game theoretic analysis shows that status quo discovery rules make this a no-risk proposition for litigants, and that they will employ the procedure as a rational strategy in litigation. Courts and legislatures can, with minimal interventions, ensure that blind expertise becomes the new gold standard in litigation, just as it is in science. This Article explores blind expertise as a worthwhile reform to the system of medical malpractice liability in particular and suggests that the procedure may be viable in many other contexts beyond litigation, wherever laypersons must rely upon experts that may be biased.
Posted by Adam Kolber on 07/28/2009 at 08:40 AM | Permalink | Comments (0) | TrackBack (0)
The New York Times recently ran this interesting article on the future of artificial intelligence. Some people are starting/continuing to worry about what happens when, say, speech synthesis gets close enough to real human speech that bad guys start using the technology to cheat or otherwise victimize people. Oh, and autonomous killing machines. They're worried about that, too.
Here's an excerpt from the NYT article about a recent meeting on the subject organized by the Association for the Advancement of Artificial Intelligence at the Asilomar Conference Grounds in Monterey Bay, CA:
The A.A.A.I. report will try to assess the possibility of “the loss of human control of computer-based intelligences.” It will also grapple, Dr. Horvitz said, with socioeconomic, legal and ethical issues, as well as probable changes in human-computer relationships. How would it be, for example, to relate to a machine that is as intelligent as your spouse?
Dr. Horvitz said the panel was looking for ways to guide research so that technology improved society rather than moved it toward a technological catastrophe. Some research might, for instance, be conducted in a high-security laboratory.
The meeting on artificial intelligence could be pivotal to the future of the field. Paul Berg, who was the organizer of the 1975 Asilomar meeting and received a Nobel Prize for chemistry in 1980, said it was important for scientific communities to engage the public before alarm and opposition becomes unshakable.
“If you wait too long and the sides become entrenched like with G.M.O.,” he said, referring to genetically modified foods, “then it is very difficult. It’s too complex, and people talk right past each other.”
Posted by Adam Kolber on 07/27/2009 at 09:39 AM | Permalink | Comments (0) | TrackBack (0)
No neuroscience here. But Ilya Somin at the Volokh Conspiracy offers this modest proposal related to the bar exam. Make those who create and administer the bar exam take it and pass it each year. From his post:
Of course, few if any bar exam officials or state bar leaders could pass the bar exam without extensive additional study (some might fail even with it). That's because, as anyone who has taken a bar exam knows, they test knowledge of thousands of arcane legal rules that only a tiny minority of practicing lawyers ever use. This material isn't on the exam because you can't be a competent lawyer if you don't know it. It's there so as to make it more difficult to pass, thereby diminishing competition for current bar association members (the people whose representatives, not coincidentally, control the bar exam process in most states - either directly or through their lobbying efforts). Effectively, bar exams screen out potential lawyers who are bad at memorization or who don't have the time and money to take a bar prep course or spend weeks on exam preparation.
My proposed reform wouldn't fully solve this problem. But it could greatly diminish it. If bar exam board members and bar association leaders were required to take and pass the exam every year, they would have strong incentives to reduce the amount of petty trivia that is tested. After all, anything they include on the exam is something they themselves will have to memorize! As prominent practicing lawyers, however, they presumably are already familiar with those laws that are so basic that any attorney has to know them; by limiting the exam to those rules, they can minimize their own preparation time. In this way, the material tested on bar exams might be limited to the relatively narrow range of legal rules that the average practicing lawyer really does need to know.
Posted by Adam Kolber on 07/27/2009 at 09:29 AM | Permalink | Comments (0) | TrackBack (0)
In a comment to an earlier post on the Neuroethics & Law Blog (see here), Matthew Lena has posted a comment requesting help with research on Gage's life. See the comment for more information if you'd like to help out with this interesting research project.
Posted by Adam Kolber on 07/27/2009 at 09:04 AM | Permalink | Comments (0) | TrackBack (0)
The MacArthur Law and Neuroscience Project has added a blog here. I've also added it to the blogroll. Welcome!
Posted by Adam Kolber on 07/27/2009 at 09:00 AM | Permalink | Comments (0) | TrackBack (0)
Zack Lynch's new book, "The Neuro Revolution," is now available. Some of you may know that Zack writes the very interesting Brain Waves blog and was a speaker at the First Annual Meeting of the Neuroethics Society. On his blog, he often focuses on the business side of advances in the neurosciences, a topic that surely warrants our attention. I have yet to read the book but will soon receive a copy of it. Here are some promotional materials for the book:
Published by St. Martin's Press
History has already progressed through an agricultural revolution, an industrial revolution, and an information revolution. THE NEURO REVOLUTION: How Brain Science Is Changing Our World (St. Martin's Press) by Zack Lynch foretells a fast approaching fourth epoch that will yet again transform how humans live, work and play. Neurotechnology-new tools for both understanding and influencing our brains including brain imaging-is now being applied not only to breakthrough medicine but to almost every aspect of human endeavor, from financial markets to law enforcement to politics to advertising and marketing, artistic expression, warfare, and even to religious belief.By telling us the stories behind the brilliant people leading this worldwide revolution, taking us into their laboratories, boardrooms and courtrooms, Zack Lynch illuminates a unique, insider's glimpse into the startling future now arriving at our doorstep. The insights and revelations within THE NEURO REVOLUTION will foster wonder, debate, and in some cases consternation. Above all, though, they need to be understood by those who will be most affected-all of us.
THE NEURO REVOLUTION is the first book to provide a comprehensive view of the impact brain science will have on law, marketing, financial markets, education, national defense, religion, investing, healthcare, government and entertainment. Moreover, the book describes a new tool through which to view the future, Time's Telescope, which draws upon the history of technological revolutions to project into our common future.
ABOUT THE AUTHOR
ZACK LYNCH is the founder and executive director of the Neurotechnology Industry Organization, a global trade association representing companies involved in neuroscience, brain research centers and patient advocacy groups. He is also the co-founder of NeuroInsights, the world's leading market research firm covering the neurotechnology industry.
Please visit The Neuro Revolution website for more advance praise, information on book tour, or to purchase the book.
The Neuro Revolution
How Brain Science is Changing Our World
By Zack Lynch with Byron Laursen
Publication Date: July 19, 2009
St. Martin's Press // Trade Hardcover
0-312-37862-9 // $25.99 // 256 pages
Posted by Adam Kolber on 07/23/2009 at 07:41 PM | Permalink | Comments (0) | TrackBack (0)
critiques the analogy made by some animal rights theorists between granting rights to "marginal" humans (e.g., mentally incapable adults and infants) and bestowing rights to animals. Moving Beyond Animal Rights: A Legal/Contractualist Critique . . . explores current and historical arguments for granting some rights to mentally incapable humans. The article asserts that none of the major theories of rights for marginal humans are useful analogies for arguments for animal rights.
Posted by Adam Kolber on 07/23/2009 at 12:32 PM | Permalink | Comments (1) | TrackBack (0)
![]() ![]() |
Article
Article
Article
Posted by Adam Kolber on 07/22/2009 at 10:39 AM | Permalink | Comments (0) | TrackBack (0)
An ABA representative recently sent me news about (and will soon send me a copy of) "Criminal Mental Health and Disability Law, Evidence and Testimony". Below, I've posted some information about the book from its website (available here):
Criminal Mental Health and Disability Law, Evidence and Testimony: A Comprehensive Reference Manual for Lawyers, Judges and Criminal Justice Professionals is the first book to examine in detail the legal relationships that link criminal justice, mental health, and disability discrimination law. Regular: $110
Chapters include:
"This unique Reference Manual… [explains] what the law requires for defendants and inmates with disabilities, what legal remedies are available to correct injustices, and how our legal system can better protect their rights.” |
Co-sponsored by ABA Commission on Mental and Physical Disability Law and the ABA Criminal Justice Section
Posted by Adam Kolber on 07/19/2009 at 08:12 PM | Permalink | Comments (0) | TrackBack (0)
In the Popular Press: Cut-Prone Fighters Turn to Surgery to Limit Bleeding The Next Hacking Frontier: Your Brain? Neuroscience: Is There Anything It Can’t Do? The neuroscience of an unwanted limb 'The
myth of the chemical cure' Gibbon sings 'door-slamming' tune 'Honest Joes' and cheaters unmasked in brain scans Monkeys And Humans Use Parallel Mechanism To Recognize
Faces The Program in Ethics and Brain
Sciences (PEBS) is a collaborative effort of
the Johns Hopkins Berman Institute of Bioethics and
the Johns Hopkins Brain Sciences Institute. The
goal of PEBS is to ensure that research in brain science
proceeds with an informed understanding of ethical issues,
and that philosophical and empirical analyses of the advances
in brain research proceed with an informed understanding of
the science. With the PEBS Neuroethics News Roundup, we
will present a weekly survey of the most pertinent and
thought-provoking new articles from both peer-reviewed
academic journals and the popular press. Please
note:
if this email is not helpful to you or If you know anyone
else who may be interested in receiving this email, contact
us and we will adjust our mailing list. Thank you! Johns
Hopkins Berman Institute of Bioethics | 201 North Charles,
Suite 1701 Archived
editions of PEBS Neuroethics News Round Up
The New York Times July 10 2009
Sunday Book Review: YOU ARE HERE Why We Can Find Out Way
to the Moon But Get Lost in the Mall
The New York Times July 10 2009
Wired Science July 9 2009
Stanford Center for Law & the Biosciences Blog
Mind Hacks July 13 2009
BBC News July 15 2009
BBC News July 15 2009
Brain wiring creates false memories
New Scientist July 15 2009
New Scientist July 13 2009
Number Of Patients With Dementia On The Rise
Science Daily July 13 2009
Science Daily July 12 2009
Baltimore, MD 21201 | Office: 410-625-7871
Contact Us: [email protected], [email protected]
www.bioethicsinstitute.org/neuroethics
Posted by Johns Hopkins Berman Institute of Bioethics on 07/16/2009 at 02:41 PM | Permalink | Comments (0) | TrackBack (0)
New Scientist article here.
Posted by Adam Kolber on 07/16/2009 at 02:17 PM | Permalink | Comments (0) | TrackBack (0)
Well, actually, it's a daguerreotype. From an LA Times article:
Massachusetts photographers have unearthed the only known image of legendary brain-injury patient Phineas Gage, a daguerreotype showing the former railroad worker sitting in repose and holding the nearly 4-foot-long iron rod that pierced his brain without killing him.
Contemporary accounts suggest that Gage's personality was dramatically altered because he was disfigured in the accident, but the new image, to be published online next week in the Journal of the History of the Neurosciences, shows a relatively handsome man -- confirming the belief of most experts that damage to his brain accounted for the changed personality.
I like how the second paragraph above connects the photo to our understanding of the famous case and not just to our morbid curiosity. If you click on the small image of Gage in the L.A. Times article, you'll see a larger version.
Hat tip: Mind Hacks (which has some more to say about the photo).
Update: An emailer notes that a daguerreotype is considered a type of photograph. So the first sentence of this post may have been misleading.
Posted by Adam Kolber on 07/16/2009 at 01:29 PM | Permalink | Comments (2) | TrackBack (0)
Here is a recent study in the European Journal of Social Psychology:
Checkmate? The role of gender stereotypes in the ultimate intellectual sport
|
Anne Maass *, Claudio D'Ettole, Mara Cadinu |
University of Padova, Italy |
*Correspondence to Anne Maass, DPSS, Universitò di Padova, Via Venezia, 8, 35139 Padova, Italy.
Abstract |
Women are surprisingly underrepresented in the chess world, representing less that 5% of registered tournament players worldwide and only 1% of the world's grand masters. In this paper it is argued that gender stereotypes are mainly responsible for the underperformance of women in chess. Forty-two male-female pairs, matched for ability, played two chess games via Internet. When players were unaware of the sex of opponent (control condition), females played approximately as well as males. When the gender stereotype was activated (experimental condition), women showed a drastic performance drop, but only when they were aware that they were playing against a male opponent. When they (falsely) believed to be playing against a woman, they performed as well as their male opponents. In addition, our findings suggest that women show lower chess-specific self-esteem and a weaker promotion focus, which are predictive of poorer chess performance. (Hat tip: The Situationist) |
Posted by Adam Kolber on 07/13/2009 at 06:48 AM | Permalink | Comments (0) | TrackBack (0)
This New York Times article tells the story of ultra-long-distance runner, Diane Van Deren. In 1997, she had a lobectomy to treat epilepsy. While most of the article discusses her truly amazing athletic ability, the article briefly suggests that the removal of part of her brain has actually enhanced her ability to run:
“When she is running, it helps her,” Don Gerber, a clinical neuropsychologist who has worked extensively with Van Deren, said of the hole in Van Deren’s brain. “In the rest of her life, it does not.”
. . .
Van Deren can no longer read maps. Telling her to go five miles, turn left, then right, then left is a confusing algorithm. She rarely runs a race without a wrong turn. “Everyone knows not to follow me now,” she said.
Gerber, who works at Craig Hospital, a rehabilitation hospital in Englewood, Colo., for people with brain or spinal-cord injuries, said that Van Deren “can go hours and hours and have no idea how long it’s been.” Her mind carries little dread for how far she is from the finish. She does not track her pace, even in training. Her gauge is the sound of her feet on the trail.
“It’s a kinesthetic melody that she hits,” Gerber said. “And when she hits it, she knows she’s running well.”
The article also makes clear that she was already an impressive athlete before the surgery. So if there is any enhancement effect, it's hardly clear. And, of course, the surgery has also increased the likelihood that she'll make a wrong turn on one of her 100+ mile treks. So, it may not be a net enhancement, if it's an enhancement in any sense. Still, it's interesting to consider the possibility that one could enhance athletic performance by removing a part of the brain that makes us fearful of ultra-long athletic activity.
Posted by Adam Kolber on 07/09/2009 at 05:47 AM | Permalink | Comments (0) | TrackBack (0)